See data and maps.

Plain text

Nasledskova, P. and T. Philippova (2022). “Agreement of adpositions with the absolutive/nominative argument”. In: Typological Atlas of the Languages of Daghestan (TALD). Ed. by M. Daniel, K. Filatov, T. Maisak, G. Moroz, T. Mukhin, C. Naccarato and S. Verhees. Moscow: Linguistic Convergence Laboratory, NRU HSE. DOI: 10.5281/zenodo.6807070. http://lingconlab.ru/dagatlas.

BibTeX

@incollection{nasledskova2022,
  title = {Agreement of adpositions with the absolutive/nominative argument},
  author = {Polina Nasledskova and Tatiana Philippova},
  year = {2022},
  editor = {Michael Daniel and Konstantin Filatov and Timur Maisak and George Moroz and Timofey Mukhin and Chiara Naccarato and Samira Verhees},
  publisher = {Linguistic Convergence Laboratory, NRU HSE},
  address = {Moscow},
  booktitle = {Typological Atlas of the Languages of Daghestan (TALD)},
  url = {http://lingconlab.ru/dagatlas},
  doi = {10.5281/zenodo.6807070},
}

1 Introduction

Adpositions in a subset of East Caucasian languages have morphological slots for class (gender and number) agreement with the absolutive/nominative argument of the clause.1 This is illustrated in example (1) from Godoberi, where one can clearly see that the adposition agrees with the absolutive/nominative argument rather than with its dependent noun phrase.

  1. Godoberi (Andic) (A. Kibrik et al. 1996: 88)
    waša hamaχi-č’u w=oχut’u wu=na
    boy.m.abs donkey.n-cont m=after m=go.pst
    ‘The boy went after the donkey’

Here, the postposition w=oχut’u ‘after’ has a dependent noun ‘donkey’ which belongs to the neuter class, but the agreement prefix on the postposition is masсuline, determined by the only masculine noun in the clause, which is the absolutive/nominative argument waša ‘boy’.

Agreement of adpositions with the absolutive/nominative is attested in languages of all branches of East Caucasian, except for Nakh and Khinalug. Table 1 summarizes data on agreeing postpositions in every language that has it, grouped by branch. One can see that this phenomenon is quite common in Dargwa languages and in Lak, less prominent in Avar-Andic and quite marginal in Lezgic and Tsezic.

Table 1. Number and percentage of postpositions agreeing in gender in East Caucasian languages

Branch Language Number of agreeing postpositions Percentage of agreeing postpositions
Dargwa Kubachi 8 62%
Akusha 15 52%
Mehweb 11 85%
Itsari 12 44%
Sanzhi 11 69%
Median 11 62%
Lak Lak 14 48%
Avar-Andic Avar 10 31%
Andi (Rikvani) 1 10%
Godoberi 4 25%
Tindi 1 5%
Bagvalal 5 19%
Chamalal 3 16%
Botlikh 4 27%
Median 4 19%
Lezgic Tsakhur 2 8%
Archi 1 6%
Median 1.5 7%
Tsezic Khwarshi 2 12%
Bezhta 1 4%
Hunzib 1 6%
Tsez 1 6%
Hinuq 1 5%
Median 1 6%

Agreement slots may be prefixal, suffixal, prefixal and suffixal, or (rarely) infixal, as shown in (1), (2), (3) and (4), respectively. Note that the infixal strategy is rare and should probably be taken as a special case of the prefixal strategy.

  1. Avar (Avar-Andic) (Rudnev 2020: 833)
    školal-da ask’o-w jasaɬ was w-uχana
    school.n.obl-loc near-m girl.f.erg boy.m.abs m-beat.pst
    ‘The girl beat the boy up near the school.’
  2. Sanzhi Dargwa (Dargwa)2 (Forker 2020: 155)
    qal-la r-i-r=da
    house-gen f-in-f=1
    ‘I (fem.) am inside the house.’
  3. Archi (Lezgic) (Bond, Chumakina 2016: 73)
    goroχči ba-qˁa haˁtər-če-qˁa-k
    rolling.stone.iii.sg.abs iii.sg-come.pfv river.iv-sg.obl-inter-lat
    e‹b›q’en
    ‹iii.SG›up_to
    ‘The rolling stone went up to the river. (The stone is near the river, but dry.)’

Table 2 summarizes the distribution of the four marking strategies across languages and postpositions. One can see that Tsezic postpositions only have prefixal agreement markers. Prefixal marking is also the dominant option in the Andic languages — it is the only pattern found in all languages of the branch exhibiting this phenomenon. In contrast, most agreeing postpositions in Avar and the Dargwa languages have the designated slot in the suffix. Finally, agreement in both the prefix and the suffix is only found in Dargwa languages. It is also interesting to note that there are on average 1–2 postpositions with the agreement marker in the prefix (30 postpositions in 15 languages), prefix and suffix (7 postpositions in 4 languages) or infix (3 postpositions in 3 languages) for each language where these slots are attested. By contrast, there are on average 9 postpositions with agreement markers in the suffixal slot in each language where this slot is attested (66 postpositions in 8 languages). In other words, suffixal agreement is the most productive pattern.

Table 2. Position of the agreement slot across languages and postpositions

Position No. of languages No. of postpositions Percentage of all
prefix 15 (6 Andic, 4 Dargwa, 5 Tsezic and Lak) 30 28%
suffix 8 (Avar, Lak, 5 Dargwa, 1 Lezgic) 66 62%
prefix and suffix 4 (4 Dargwa) 7 7%
infix 3 (2 Andic, 1 Lezgic) 3 3%

Summarizing the discussion, the phenomenon of adpositional agreement with the absolutive/nominative is widely attested in East Caucasian languages. However, it is rarely found cross-linguistically. In his book on adpositions, Hagège (2010: 175–176) provides examples of adpositions agreeing with the nominative from Avar, regional Southern Italian dialects and Iwaidja (Non-Pama Nyungan, Northern Territory, Australia). In Iwaidja, the agreement marker is on the adposition, but unlike in East Caucasian, Iwaidjan adpositions agree with the nominative in person and number, and not in gender and number (5).

  1. Iwaidja (Evans 2000: 131) via (Hagège 2010: 176)
    buɲaɹi mirinayai ɹaga gunag
    3.sg.without crocodile dem place
    ‘this place has got no crocodile’

We would like to note a certain discrepancy between the Italian dialect of Ripano (spoken in Ripatransone, Marche) — one of the few languages that exhibits pervasive agreement with the nominative (including on adverbs) and East Caucasian: while it is true that adverbs (including spatial ones) agree in gender and number with the absolutive/nominative in both cases, (6 b), Ripano Italian prepositions apparently agree with their nominal dependent, (6 a).

  1. Ripano Italian

    1. comma

      sembra

      er-e

      chepit-e

      d-e

      sol-e

      chə

      as

      always

      be.impf-3.f

      understood-sg.f

      of-sg.f

      alone-sg.f

      that

      refl

      vəliev-u

      scallà

      n-e

      occ-e

      dop-a

      magnat-a

      wanted.impf-3.sg.m

      warm.inf

      indef-sg.f

      drop-sg.f

      of

      wine.sg.m

      after-n

      eating-n

      ‘As usual, she had understood that he wanted to warm up some wine for himself after dinner.’

      (Lambertelli 2003: 78) via (D’Alessandro 2020: 265)

    2. Ndovu

      va?

      where.m

      go.3.sg

      ‘Where is he going?’

      (D’Alessandro 2017: 28)

This pattern is also marginally found at least with one postposition of Mehweb Dargwa, cf. example (7) where the prefixal slot exhibits agreement with the dependent NP and the suffixal slot takes the value corresponding to the absolutive argument. In addition, Forker (2013: 388–389) reports on the possibility of agreement with the dependent for the only agreeing Hinuq postposition -oƛƛo ‘in the middle of’, see example (8). Otherwise, to the best of our knowledge, East Caucasian agreeing postpositions behave just as agreeing adverbs, that is, they agree with the absolutive/nominative.

  1. Mehweb Dargwa (Dargwa) (courtesy of Michael Daniel)
    heš šara=gʷa ʁir-me-la d-ajcana-b
    this lake=ptcl rock-pl-gen npl-amidst-n
    ‘This lake is amidst rocks.’
  2. Hinuq (Tsezic) (Forker 2013: 389)
    hes bahadur=no ∅-oƛex-iš elu-de b-oƛƛo goɬa
    one knight.i=and i-appear-pst we.obl-aloc hpl-in.the.middle be.ptcp
    guħ-ƛ’o-r
    hill-sup-lat
    ‘And one knight appeared on a hill that was in the middle of us.’

The specific feature of adpositional agreement with the absolutive/nominative that we explore here is not mentioned in WALS: it only contains a chapter on person marking on adpositions (Bakker 2013), which is apparently attested in a greater number of the world’s languages. This feature is tangential to our purposes, however, since it only covers cases where adpositions bear a person affix whose value is dependent on the person (and, sometimes, number) of the adposition’s immediate complement; this phenomenon is not attested in East Caucasian.

In what follows we classify the languages of Daghestan according to the presence/absence of adpositions agreeing with the absolutive/nominative arguments.

2 Results

Due to the relatively small number of agreeing adpositions in East Caucasian, we decided to simply map the presence of such adpositions in a language. Thus, we have two values: attested for languages that are reported to have adpositions agreeing with the absolutive/nominative argument and not attested for languages for which we have not found any evidence for agreeing adpositions. The distribution of the languages by values is as follows:

Table 3. Adpositional agreement with the nominative in Daghestan

Value N
Number of languages where adpositions agreeing with the nominative are attested 20
Number of languages where adpositions agreeing with the nominative are not attested 23

They are mapped on Map 1.

Map 2 shows the percentage of postpositions that agree with the absolutive/nominative for all East Caucasian languages and their neighbors.

In Dargwa languages, agreement of postpositions with the absolutive/nominative is very productive, likely due to the fact that class agreement markers are part of the spatial inflection system in these languages, specifically, they are used to signal essive. In other branches (Tsezic, Andic) only a small number of postpositions (cognate within each branch) have agreement morphology, indicating that in these languages this type of agreement is not productive and likely inherited. Avar could be characterized as an intermediate case between Dargwa and Tsezic/Andic: postpositions derived from nouns have a suffixal agreement marker in the essive form (similarly to nouns), whereas other postpositions do not have it. Lak adpositions only have agreement when taking the lative form (built on the gender marker and an orientation morpheme), while their essive forms, built just on the gender marker no longer inflect; the same pattern is observed for Lak nouns in the spatial cases. Thus, the situation in Lak is somewhat similar to that found in Dargwa and Avar, yet it is more complicated.

Additionally, Figure 1 maps which positions of the agreement slot are found on adpositions of a given language.

Figure 1. Possible positions of agreement slots on adpositions, by language

Finally, Map 3 shows the share of the agreeing postpositions that can be used independently (as adverbs) in the language. In most languages with adpositional agreement (14 out of 20), all agreeing adpositions are identical to adverbs. In the three languages that fall into the intermediate category (Bagvalal, Chamalal, Lak), a minority of the agreeing postpositions are deverbal, hence they do not admit adverbial use. Archi, Tsez and Tindi have just one agreeing postposition each: in Archi and Tsez it is known to be deverbal, while the origin of the Tindi lexeme meaning ‘up to’ is unclear, but it is likely to be deverbal too, given its semantics.

3 Distribution

Adpositional agreement with the absolutive/nominative in languages of Daghestan is attested only in a subset of East Caucasian languages. Among these languages, such postpositions are most typical of Dargwa, Lak, and Avar-Andic languages: they are reported for all languages of these branches except for Akhvakh and Karata (Andic) and Kaitag (Dargwa). Outside these branches, agreeing postpositions are attested in the Tsezic languages, which are both genealogically and geographically close to Avar-Andic languages, and in two Lezgic languages: Archi and Tsakhur. In Archi, the only agreeing postposition is deverbal (A. E. Kibrik 1977: 176), so the (infixal) agreement slot is likely inherited from its diachronic source. The two agreeing postpositions attested in Tsakhur also function as adverbs and are part of a small group of underived adverbs that have an agreement suffix (A. E. Kibrik 1999: 357).

List of glosses

1 — first person; 3 — third person; abs — absolutive; aloc — animate location; cont — contessive; dem — demonstrative; erg — ergative; f — feminine; gen — genitive; hpl — human plural; i — class I; iii — class III; impf — imperfective; indef — indefinite; inf — infinitive; inter — within (a solid object); iv — class IV; lat — lative; loc — locative; m — masculine; n — neuter; npl — non-human plural; obl — oblique; pfv — perfective; pl — plural; pst — past; ptcl — particle; ptcp — participle; refl — reflexive; sg — singular; sup — superessive

References

Bakker, D. (2013). Person marking on adpositions. In M. S. Dryer, M. Haspelmath (Eds.), The World Atlas of Language Structures Online. Leipzig: Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology. Retrieved from http://wals.info/chapter/48
Bond, O., Chumakina, M. (2016). Agreement domains and targets. In C. Bond Oliver, D. Brown (Eds.), Archi: Complexities of agreement in cross-theoretical perspective (pp. 43–76). Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Creissels, D. (2009). Uncommon patterns of core term marking and case terminology. Lingua, 119(3), 445–459.
D’Alessandro, R. (2017). When you have too many features: Auxiliaries, agreement and clitics in Italian varieties. Glossa: A Journal of General Linguistics, 2(1), Article 50. Retrieved from https://doi.org/10.5334/gjgl.102
D’Alessandro, R. (2020). Agreement across the board: Topic agreement in Ripano. In P. W. Smith, J. Mursell, K. Hartmann (Eds.), Agree to Agree: Agreement in the Minimalist Programme (pp. 235–270). Berlin: Language Science Press. Retrieved from DOI:10.5281/zenodo.3541757
Daniel, M., Ganenkov, D. (2009). Case marking in Daghestanian. In A. Malchukov, A. Spencer (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of case (pp. 668–685). Oxford University Press.
Evans, N. (2000). Iwaidjan, a very un-Australian language family. Linguistic Typology, 4(1), 91–142.
Forker, D. (2013). A grammar of Hinuq. Berlin/Boston: De Gruyter Mouton.
Forker, D. (2020). A grammar of Sanzhi Dargwa. Berlin: Language Science Press.
Hagège, C. (2010). Adpositions. Oxford: Oxford University Press.
Kibrik, A. E. (1977). Opyt strukturnogo opisanija arčinskogo jazyka. Tom II. Taksonomičeskaja grammatika [Structural description of Archi. Volume II. Taxonomic grammar]. Moscow: Izdatelʹstvo Moskovskogo universiteta.
Kibrik, A. E. (1997). Beyond subject and object: Toward a comprehensive relational typology. Linguistic Typology, 1(3), 279–346.
Kibrik, A. E. (1999). Èlementy caxurskogo jazyka v tipologičeskom osveščenii [Elements of Tsakhur grammar in a typological perspective]. Moscow: Nasledie.
Kibrik, A., Tatevosov, S., Eulenberg, A. (1996). Godoberi. Munich/Newcastle: Lincom Europa.
Lambertelli, G. (2003). Elementi di fonetica, ortografia e grammatica della parlata ripana con doppia versione di J’sè d lu Rrotı̀”. Ripatransone: Giulio Lambertelli Editore.
Rudnev, P. (2020). Agreeing adpositions in Avar and the directionality-of-valuation debate. Linguistic Inquiry, 51(4), 829–844.

  1. For the purposes of this chapter we unite agreement of adpositions with the absolutive argument with that with the nominative argument. This unification is valid at least for two reasons: 1) there are languages (e.g., Italian dialects) exhibiting agreement with the nominative which patterns similarly to the agreement with the absolutive found in East Caucasian; 2) some East Caucasian experts (cf. e.g., (A. E. Kibrik 1997); (Daniel, Ganenkov 2009); (Creissels 2009: 448)) prefer the term nominative over absolutive for the morphologically unmarked S/P case.↩︎

  2. The prefixal gender marker in Sanzhi Dargwa looks like the ones found on verbs, whereas the suffixal marker closely resembles the gender marking obligatory on essive nouns, see Forker (2020: 414–415) for relevant examples. The absolutive argument triggering agreement is omitted in (3).↩︎